The Reading
Matrix
Vol3, No.3, November
2003
Interactive Hypertext
and the Development of ESL Students' Reading
Skills
Loretta F. Kasper
Abstract
Recent research
suggests that hypertext can provide an effective
tool for developing reading skills. Because
hypertext is a relatively new textual medium,
and because it is likely to become more dominant
in the future, research is needed so that both
reading instructors and students may be empowered
to use hypertext to its full advantage. This
paper reports on the results of a two-year
study designed to assess the effects of different
types of hypertext on the development of ESL
students' reading skills.
In a society that places
increasing emphasis on the importance of information
and communication, strong reading skills are
essential not only for students' academic success,
but also for their social and economic advancement.
As Grabe and Stoller (2002) assert, "As we enter
a new century, productive and educated citizens
will require even stronger literacy abilities
(including both reading and writing) in increasingly
larger numbers of societal settings" (p.
1). Unfortunately, many college students, especially
those for whom English is a second language,
do not read well enough to ensure comprehension
nor to meet minimum competency standards set
by institutions of higher education. For example,
during the 1999-2000 academic year at my institution,
77% of the ESL students who, after one semester
of high intermediate instruction, needed to continue
in developmental English did so because they
had failed the end-of-semester reading examinations.
Clearly these students need much support in building
the critical reading skills necessary for higher-level
learning.
The results of my own, as well as other recent
research, suggest that through its use of interactive
hypertext, Internet technology can provide an
effective tool for developing reading skills
(Kasper, 2002; Lomicka, 1998; Soe, Koki, Chang,
2000; Tierney, Kieffer, Whalin, Desai, Moss,
Harris, & Hopper, 1997; Warschauer, 1999).
This paper reports the results of a two-year
study designed to explore and assess the possible
effects of hypertext on the development of ESL
students' reading skills.
HYPERTEXT AND READING SKILLS In contrast to traditional print, in which information
is presented in a linear fashion (i.e., in predefined
sequences), the presentation of information in
hypertext is nonlinear and represented in a semantic
network in which multiple related sections of
the text are connected to each other. By following
links through the sections of the text, the reader
actively engages with that text, choosing a path
that is most relevant to his or her needs or
interests. Well-designed hypertext systems can
facilitate interaction between readers and texts
(Rouet, Levonen, Dillon, & Spiro, 1996),
thereby enhancing comprehension and building
critical reading skills. Because hypertext provides
easy access to multiple cross-references on related
topics across several documents, or screens,
it fosters a nonlinear and flexible pattern of
exploration and discovery that encourages a natural
juxtaposition of ideas presented (Tierney et
al., 1997) and helps to promote cognitive flexibility
necessary for the integration and consolidation
of knowledge gleaned from a variety of sources
(Mishra, Spiro, & Feltovich, 1996).
Following new and different links during subsequent
online sessions encourages student readers to
develop strategies for how to approach text content,
as they call upon appropriate relevant schemata,
or background knowledge for how to interpret
that content. Reading hypertext is a naturally
dynamic, recursive, and integrated process, one
that provides multiple opportunities for students
to acquire, test and reframe knowledge through
cognitive reconstruction of text, intertextual
analysis and exposure to varied perspectives
on issues. Thus, hypertext may promote increased
comprehension through the elaboration and integration
of new information into the existing knowledge
network as readers create and expand the cognitive
map that guides their construction of meaning.
Although nonlinear hypertext can offer students
many benefits, Rouet and Levonen (1996) advise
that without overt instruction in how to navigate
hypertext effectively, students may become lost
in a sea of information, potentially experiencing
cognitive overload. Gillingham (1996) suggests
that attention to text, task, and context is
necessary for effective comprehension of hypertext.
Foltz (1996) cautions that hypertext may present
a problem for students with poor reading skills
because it causes an additional processing load
by making the reader responsible for navigating
the text. Having to choose where to go next can
take students' attention away from processing
the text, with the possible result that they
generate fewer hypotheses as they read, making
it harder for them to integrate the information
presented. Foltz' work pointed to two key factors
in hypertext comprehension: (1) the coherence
of the text and (2) how the reader's goals affected
strategies used. Each of these researchers points
to the need for extended research to elucidate
the role of hypertext in building reading skills.
Currently research on hypertext suggests that
while it has the potential to be a powerful tool
for building reading skills, without proper instruction,
hypertext can lead to information overload and
confusion, especially in developmental readers.
Because hypertext is a relatively new textual
medium, and because it is likely to become more
dominant in the future, research is needed that
explores its features and their effects on reading
comprehension and performance. In this way, both
reading instructors and students may be empowered
to use hypertext to its full advantage.
The study reported here had several objectives:
(1) to design different types of hypertext and
then assess and evaluate the effects of each
on reading comprehension and performance, (2)
to collect feedback from students about which
types of hypertext they believe are most helpful,
(3) to apply results to revise and refine hypertexts
to help students improve reading skills more
efficiently, (4) to apply results to teach students
more effective strategies for reading and comprehending
all texts, whether hypertext or traditional print,
and (5) to construct and make available on the
Internet a body of interactive online texts that
can be used to develop students' reading skills.
THE ESL COURSE AND CURRICULUM
The study was conducted
over a period of four semesters and involved
100 ESL students. The students who participated
in the study were enrolled in a high intermediate
course called “Developing
Fluency in Reading and Writing for ESL Students” (ESL
91), which is the first of a two course developmental
sequence in reading and writing. High intermediate
here is defined as an entry level TOEFL score
of approximately 425. The ESL 91 class meets
six hours per week in three two-hour blocks for
twelve weeks. Students meet in the computer lab
each week for one of those two-hour blocks. My
course follows a sustained content curriculum
(see Pally, 2000) that develops ESL students'
literacy skills through an activity called focus
discipline research (Kasper, 2002a, 2002b).
To help my ESL students build key literacy skills,
I have designed a curriculum of technology-enhanced
focus discipline research that is based on the
principles of sustained content study. An extensive
description of the focus discipline curriculum
is beyond the scope of this paper (interested
readers are referred to Kasper, 2002b); however,
briefly defined, a focus discipline is a subject
area (e.g., psychology, biology) that individual
students choose to research extensively over
the course of the semester. I ask my students
to choose a focus discipline from the ten subject
areas contained in their textbook. 2 Students
base their choice on personal interest and/or
college major, and because students have chosen to
do extensive research in that discipline, they
are actively invested in a learning experience
that is personally meaningful and important.
TECHNOLOGY AS A TOOL FOR INSTRUCTION
My ESL students hone English language skills,
build their overall knowledge base, and develop
literacy skills through their use of text-based
computer-mediated communication, intensive reading
and research using Internet hypertext documents,
and their production of written essays and individual
and group research projects based on their research
efforts. As noted earlier, my ESL class meets
six hours per week in three two-hour blocks for
twelve weeks. Students meet in the computer lab
each week for one of those two-hour blocks. Many
of my ESL students have computers at home; those
that do not have additional access in the college
during open computer lab times.
My focus discipline research curriculum makes
extensive use of group collaboration within the
physical classroom, where students work together
to share and build knowledge with others studying
the same focus discipline. Internet technologies
have allowed me to enhance and expand this curriculum
within and beyond the physical classroom. I have
incorporated a course web site at http://kccesl.tripod.com/ and
an online course component, called “ Interdisciplinary
English and the Internet ,” on Blackboard.com.
3 I post weekly computer lab assignments to the
Announcements area of the Blackboard course.
Computer lab work consists of a variety of different
activities, from Internet research to online
reader response exercises to online practice
reading and writing tests.
I design general class activities to teach students
vocabulary and language structures and to provide
them with day-to-day practice in complex interdisciplinary
texts. These general class lessons provide guided
instruction on how to dissect a text, search
for clues to meaning, and compose cogent responses
to inferential questions and essay prompts. Because
students do a good deal of Internet research
as part of the course, they must search for,
evaluate and read an extensive number of hypertexts.
As Rouet and Levonen (1996) note, although hypertext
can offer students many benefits in terms of
developing reading skills, without instruction
in how to navigate hypertext effectively, students
may become lost in a sea of information, potentially
experiencing cognitive overload. Moreover, students
need to be able to discern which texts are reliable
and valid for the purposes of their research.
Guided practice that familiarizes students with
criteria for evaluating web sites and that takes
them through a hypertext document facilitates
their acquisition of the cognitive strategies
necessary for finding, navigating and comprehending
nonlinear texts. To help my students build skills
in searching for, sorting through and evaluating
Internet texts, I give them a multi-part guided
research activity . This activity, which is posted
on the course web site, as well as on the online
Blackboard course component, teaches students
not only how to search for information on the
Internet, but also how to evaluate the resources
they find there. We do the guided research activity
in the computer lab.
Students begin by reading the text Four
Nets for Better Searching ; this text provides
suggestions for how to conduct an effective
Internet search. Students then study an Evaluation
Criteria Chart (Kapoun, 2000), which recommends
five criteria for evaluating web sites. We
go over each of the criteria, using sample
web sites on global warming, one of the subjects
studied in the course, to illustrate the principles
listed. . Finally students complete an Evaluation
Exercise that directs them to four different
web sites on global warming and asks them to
evaluate each site based on the five criteria
provided in the Evaluation Criteria Chart.
They are then asked to expand their knowledge
base through an independent Internet search.
To do this, students need to become familiar
with Internet search engines, such as Yahoo !, AltaVista , Google ,
or Vivisimo . They
must learn how to enter keywords to identify
the information they want. Then once
the Internet search engine has returned a list
of "hits" for the keyword, students
must go through the list to identify the most
appropriate and/or useful information.
The next part of the activity provides students
with additional practice in searching for and
evaluating Internet resources (see Appendix A).
Students are directed to the EPA global warming
web site. Students are guided through the information
and links found on this web site. Together we
explore and discuss all of the information presented.
Then students are asked to practice accessing
related web sites on their own. Students must
write up a short response to the information
on these sites. After completing their search,
students share the resources they have found
and their responses to them through focus discipline
group work in the classroom.
DESIGNING THE HYPERTEXTS The study reported here was conducted in class,
and the texts used were an integral part of the
course. I constructed several types of hypertexts
4 , each focusing on subject areas (e.g., environmental
science, psychology) studied in the course. Initially,
I had planned three types of hypertext conditions:
(1) glosses ,
in which links provide popup vocabulary definitions,
(2) controlled
hypertexts , in which links lead to a predetermined
and limited number of texts on the topic, (3) free
hypertexts , in which students read a main
text and then are directed to freely explore
the Internet for other texts related to the topic.
However, during the initial semester of the project,
it became clear that two additional conditions
(for a total of five) were needed: (4) controlled
hypertexts with glosses, and (5) free
hypertexts with glosses . These were designed
and incorporated into the study during the second
semester of the study. Thus, there were a total
of five hypertext conditions in the overall study.
As integral components of the ESL course, the
texts used in the study were online versions
of print texts contained in the book Interdisciplinary
English (Kasper, 1998). The selection of
glossed vocabulary items in the online versions
of the texts corresponded to those vocabulary
items highlighted, but not glossed, in the print
versions of the texts. Students reading print
versions were required to complete vocabulary
exercises in which they used the context of the
reading passage to write a definition for each
of the highlighted items. The highlighted vocabulary
items and the corresponding online glossed items
were selected because these words were likely
to be unfamiliar to students and were words that
were deemed necessary for overall comprehension
of the text. Students reading online versions
of the texts were provided with pop-up vocabulary
definitions via glosses to use technology to
help relieve some of the cognitive load associated
with reading online texts. It should be emphasized
that students read only one version,
either print or online, of any given text.
RESULTS OF THE STUDY I conducted the study as part of the ESL 91
course work during the two-hour computer lab
time. I used the hypertexts as practice reading
exams to prepare students for their end-of-semester
reading assessment. Students read each of the
hypertexts and answered comprehension questions
online. They then submitted their answers to
me via an
online form . Students were instructed to
print out a copy of their answers before submitting
them. After answers were submitted, the form
automatically took students to a page with the
answer key 5 , where they were able to compare
the answers they had submitted with those on
the answer key. Thus the activities in this study
were designed to be integral parts of the ESL
course with the goal of helping students build
both reading and writing skills. Printed texts
in the course textbook served as control texts
to assess the general effects of hypertext itself
on reading comprehension. For the printed texts,
students wrote their answers to comprehension
questions on paper and handed them into me in
class.
To receive full credit, responses needed to
demonstrate a clear understanding of the text
and the relevance of certain information to the
issues presented in the text. Initially I scored
the students' answers to each of the texts; as
a control, another faculty member, who did not
have knowledge of the specific details of the
study, also scored the answers. Students' responses
were scored based on the same criteria used in
scoring responses to the departmental reading
examination; the answers were normed to a standard
prescribing information necessary for a correct
answer. After all texts had been scored, I tabulated
the results for each of the five types of hypertexts,
as well as the print texts, which served as controls.
There was an interrater reliability of .98 on
the scoring.
The results revealed that the quality of students'
responses varied as an effect of the type of
text read (see Appendix B for sample student
responses). Controlled and controlled gloss hypertexts
resulted in better performance than either print
or free hypertexts. The most detailed answers
of all were produced by students' responses to
controlled hypertexts with glosses. When paired
with controlled hypertexts, it appears that glosses
aided students in understanding unfamiliar vocabulary,
and the extended information provided in the
controlled hypertext enabled them to form a more
comprehensive answer to the question. Students'
answers to questions on controlled hypertexts
indicated that they actively used this extended
information in forming their answers.
There was little difference
between the detail in students' responses to
gloss hypertexts, free hypertexts, free gloss
hypertexts and print hypertexts. In their responses
to questions on each of these types of text,
students primarily used the information presented
in the “main” text. Even though free
hypertexts required them to search the Internet
for an additional source of information on the
topic of the text, students tended not to use
this additional information in forming their
responses to the comprehension questions. This
result was interesting and somewhat unexpected
given the fact that students had received extensive
instruction on how to search for and evaluate
Internet texts.
Based on students' responses to comprehension
questions and feedback questionnaires, it appears
that having to search for additional information
on their own was problematic. This was true despite
the fact that they had received specific instruction
in how to search for information efficiently.
In their responses to feedback questions, students
indicated that they were unsure of which information
gleaned from the free search was necessary to
answer the question. In addition, they stated
that their searches did not necessarily produce
results that were useful in responding to each
of the questions in the comprehension exercise.
Students who were more practiced in doing Internet
searches tended to find sites that were more
useful in helping them respond to the questions.
However, even those students who were able to
find useful sites tended not to use the information
in those sites in responding to the comprehension
questions.
Students' scores on the comprehension exercises
derived from controlled hypertexts, particularly
those that contained glosses, were significantly
higher (mean=88% correct) than scores on exercises
derived from any of the other three types of
hypertext: gloss (mean = 75%), free (mean=65%),
or free gloss (mean=68%). It should be noted
that only the mean scores students attained on
gloss and controlled gloss hypertexts would have
been considered passing on the college reading
examination, where the passing score is 70%.
In contrast students' scores on comprehension
exercises derived from free, free gloss, and
print texts (mean =60%) were lower, and would
not have been considered passing on the college
reading assessment.
All forms of hypertext produced higher scores
on comprehension tests than did print texts;
this improved performance may be due to a number
of factors. Technology use encourages students
to spend more time on task, providing them with
increased opportunities to process linguistic
and content information (Kasper, 2000a). Thus,
it is possible that students in this study may
have devoted more time to the task when reading
any type of hypertext, and this increased attention
may have yielded better performance. However,
it appears that the additional information provided
by gloss and controlled hypertexts further facilitated
comprehension and so led to better scores on
comprehension exercises. In spite of the differences
in scores obtained with the various textual forms,
teasing out the specific role of hypertext in
building reading skill is difficult from comprehension
scores alone; for this reason, getting students'
feedback on the texts used in the study was critical
to providing a clearer picture of how students
used the texts and the benefits they derived
from them.
STUDENT FEEDBACK At the end of the semester, I asked students
to complete an
online feedback form , which asked for their
reactions to the different types of hypertexts
used in the study. Students were asked what they
thought of each type of text, how easy or difficult
it was to use. They were asked to indicate their
preferences among the different types of hypertexts
and to explain how/why they believed that a specific
type of hypertext was or was not useful in helping
to improve comprehension. I used their feedback
to revise and improve the hypertexts designed
for the study.
An overwhelming majority of students (95%) reported
that they found gloss hypertexts to be the most
useful for improving vocabulary comprehension;
of these gloss hypertexts, the controlled
hypertexts with glosses were rated the easiest
to use and the most effective in helping them
construct a complete answer to the comprehension
questions. For example, one student reported, “Hypertexts
were very helpful in my readings. The most helpful
were glosses and controlled hypertexts. Glosses
because if I didn't know the meaning of the word
I just easily could check it with my mouse. Controlled
because it explained more about the subject.” Because
ESL 91 focuses on developing both reading and
writing skills, students are required to produce
written essays on the topics of each of the readings
done in the course. One student noted an additional
benefit from controlled hypertext; not only was
it easier to understand, but it also provided
additional information to incorporate into the
essays he needed to write for the course. The
student said, “…the additional information from
controlled hypertexts was very helpful for improving
and spreading in our essays.”
In contrast free
hypertexts , with or without glosses, were
rated the most difficult to use by 85% of the
students in the study. One student responded
that “ Free hypertext was difficult to use,
because I use much time on search information,
and don't know the search information that
I find have to be useful or not;” while another
stated, “ I think the Free is difficult to
use because it makes me confuse to the main
text and the addition text.”
As noted earlier, students' performance on reading
comprehension exercises mirrored their preferences.
Scores on gloss hypertexts, particularly controlled
gloss hypertexts, were significantly higher than
those on free hypertexts. Students' said that
gloss hypertexts enabled them to read with greater
comprehension because these texts provided easy
access to the definitions of new vocabulary words.
This result supports the findings of Lomicka
(1998) who found that reading computer texts
with glosses may promote a deeper level of text
comprehension. Students in this study also said
that controlled hypertexts made the text clearer
by providing links to specific relevant information.
Students' preference for controlled hypertext
supports Foltz' (1996) claims that text coherence
plays a powerful role in students' comprehension
of hypertext.
Overall students disliked
free hypertext because they found it confusing.
They complained that free hypertext led to
finding too much information, making it easy
to get lost in exploring the links and forget
about the main topic. They also said sorting
through and evaluating the usefulness of all
the different definitions and opinions on the
topic was time-consuming. In addition, free
hypertext was intimidating to students who
were less experienced with the computer. These
results support the claims of both Foltz (1996)
and Rouet & Levonen (1996) and are particularly
interesting since students here received instruction
both in how to navigate hypertext and in how
to evaluate information found on the Web.
CONCLUSIONS Overall this study has provided student feedback
and performance data that support the claims
of previous researchers and has also helped to
elucidate issues that need further attention
in future studies. Student feedback and performance
data indicated that controlled hypertexts with
glosses led to the highest levels of text comprehension.
In their responses to the feedback questionnaire,
students indicated that glosses were extremely
useful in helping them to understand new words
in the text and in building their overall vocabulary.
Therefore hypertexts designed for use in developmental
reading courses should incorporate glosses.
Students in this study
indicated that they did not like using the
free hypertexts because they found them somewhat
overwhelming. The results of the study reported
here suggest that students need more extensive
training in how to navigate the Internet and
evaluate the information they find there. Given
these results future studies that carefully
examine students' exploration of free hypertexts
are needed. These studies should collect specific
data on the number and content of the sites
visited. In addition, it is possible that students
would be more comfortable working with free
hypertext if the subject addressed were one
with which they were highly familiar. It is
also possible that with increased instruction
and practice, students will experience less information
overload with online resources, and they may
become more proficient at finding and pursuing
links that lead them to more coherent, and more
useful, free hypertexts. Moreover, learning how
to evaluate the quality of information is a key
skill, not just in academic settings, but also
in life itself. To help students build this skill,
ESL courses should incorporate different textual
media (Chun & Plass, 1997) and that ask students
to critically analyze the information presented
in print, visual and hypertexts.
The primary goal of this study was to assess
the role of hypertext in developing ESL students'
reading skills through a comparison of students'
performance with different types of hypertexts.
Unfortunately because this study was conducted
as part of the actual coursework, each type of
hypertext focused on a different subject area.
It is possible that students may have found some
subjects easier or more difficult than others,
and this may have influenced their performance
and their reactions to the type of hypertext
used. Future studies might use the insights gained
from this study and design different types of
hypertexts, each of which focuses on the same
subject. This would eliminate the potential confound
of variations in difficulty due to subject area,
and so provide a clearer picture of the effects
of different types of hypertext on reading comprehension
and performance.
Despite these caveats, the results of the study
reported here can begin to provide insights into
factors that should be considered when designing
and using hypertext as a tool for reading instruction
as well as directions for future research. As
more and more materials become available online,
it is important that ESL instructors come to
recognize the potential of and learn how to use
hypertext to its full advantage as a valid tool
for instruction. When carefully designed and
incorporated as an integral part of the ESL curriculum,
Internet hypertexts can provide students with
a highly motivating textual format that not only
enriches their overall learning experience, but
also helps them develop the literacy skills they
will need to succeed in the age of information.
1 This research was
supported by a grant from the City of New York
PSC-CUNY Research Award Program.
2 The textbook Interdisciplinary English (Kasper,
1998) contains readings in linguistics, environmental
science, computer science, psychology, sociology,
business, anthropology, diet and nutrition, biology,
and mathematics.
3 The Blackboard.com course may be found at http://online.cuny.edu:8001/
4 A representative list of hypertexts developed
for this study may be found at http://kccesl.tripod.com/hypertextstudy/
5 Both linked examples are representative of
the online question forms and answer keys used
in the overall study.
References |